ORD
FORMATION IN NAT
RAL MORPHOLOGY
he figure-ground distinction between head and non-head is established by
orphosemantic and morphotactic subordination of a non-head under the head (cf.
ection 2.2). This is by far the pref
red situation in suffixation and compounding.
he onl
partial exception in suffixation is diminutive formation (and au
mentative
and pe
orative formation in other lan
ua
es), as in
o
-ie
where the suffix does
ot determine the class of the base (a t
pi
al head propert
), unless in rare instances
h a
r
-
, where the suffix transforms an adjective into a noun.
he
reference for subordination of a non-head to a head holds even in
exocentric compounds. Not only is the non-head morphosemantically subordinated
o the absent head. But subordination holds, secondarily, even among the actually
present members of exocentric compounds:
r
inat
t
n
whereas in pick-pocke
we find again non-uniformity of (secondary) head-
ood: althou
h the primar
, semantic head which desi
nates the person who picks
pockets, is not expressed, pocke
s, secondaril
, the morphotactic head which
etermines inflection, whereas s
ntacticall
pocke
r
inat
t
pic
n compounding, subordinate compound
are universally preferred ove
oordinate com
ounds which have two or more mor
hosemantic heads
cf.
underlich 1986: 241), i.e. without the clear figure-ground distinction of
ubordinate compounds. Thus in
peaker-hearer both members are of equal status,
although the plural ending attaches only to the right member, another example of
on-uniform headhood. Coordinate (or coordinative) compounds ma
a
ain be
endocentric
such as speaker-hearer
r the ad
ective
r-
also called
appositional compounds, or the
ma
be exocentric, such as morpholo
-s
ntax
nterface, where the two coordinated com
ound members have their semantic head
t
i
it
in
i
ith
nterface
of the whole noun-phrase.
More subtle
ro
erties of coordinat
compounds may differ considerably fro
anguage to language (cf. Olsen 2001): The
in
ar
r
r
f m
m
r
in
r
inat
ompounds is not grammatically determined (since all members are equipollent), but
pragmatically (e.g. the most important first) or stylistically, e.g. prosodically (e.g.
he lon
est last). The first reason explains the order of
peaker-hearer
becaus
in
uists tend to think more of the speaker than of the hearer, cf. the ter
peaker), the second explains wh
the order of the s
non
peaker-listener
n
ore difficult to reverse
istener-s
eaker
than in the case of
eaker-hearer
On the syntagmatic level, this lexical-pragmatical grading of importance in
eaker-heare
is antagonistic to the inverse morphological order of non-heads
followed by heads in English and the ma
ority of languages. The latter sequence
epresents a universal preference for heads to be on the right side of non-heads,
alled the righthand head rule by William
(1981: 248). A minority of languages has
ar
e classes of lefthand-headed compounds as well (cf. Zwanenbur
1992a, b,
Scalise 1992: 179ff, Rainer 1993: 57
. This
ecalls the suffixin
preference, whereb
suffixes are
referred to
refixes. Most
xplanations of the suffixing preference start
from the assum
tion that it is better for a word to start with the lexical basis
cf. Hall
1992), which would hold for both right-headed and left-headed compounds. Thus if
ne compares compounds, then the more valid generalisation seems to be that it is
better for complex words to end with the head. This would also explain the tendency
for prefixes not to be heads (cf. Hall 1992). An explanation for the right-hand head