OGNITIVE APPROACH TO WORD
ORMATION
3.5 Structural descriptions, creativit
and productive usa
e
Th
-
suffix, including the Stem
-z
onstruction which automaticall
comes
along with it (7.u), characterizes many independently-established words such as
a
les
oranges
as
berries
and so forth. It sanctions all these words
(section 2.5), and gives speaker-hearers a solid basis for understanding or analyzing
hem. The higher-order schemas 7.ah and 7.ai provide a somewhat lesser degree of
sanction (lesser because they are more distan
, point (c) of section 2.5). Similarly the
set of schemas 6.aa-6.ag sanction
and its relatives. The set of such
h
ma
hi
h
an
ti
n a f
rm
n
tit
t
it
tructural description (Lan
acke
1987: 428-433). The
embod
the lin
uistic
eneralities to which it conforms.
The same patterns, however, can also be used to craft, or to understand, new
r
Su
ose a
erson has never encountered the word guillotines before
but hears
someone else say it. The word guillotin
(4.w), we are supposing, is already
established. It is very easy to plug this autonomous, already-existent bipola
tr
t
r
int
-z’s e-site and allow the resultant structure to sanction the particula
pronunciation and contextual meaning of the novel word. Essentially the same thing
would happen for a person who thinks o
a group of several guillotines and co-
activates the stem and the suffixal construction to
uide his pronunciation of the new
word. This kind of usa
e is dia
rammed in 4.v. As in 7.ai, the dashed lines and
ounded corners of the boxes indicate that the structures
in this case the
m
inati
n
f
ILL
TINE
it
PL
RAL and of
I+
with
, as well as the
h
l
tr
t
r
uillotines
are not (
et) established as part of the lin
uistic s
stem.
Similarl
, the N-N compound schemas of Fi
ure 6 can be used to sanction the
formation and understandin
of novel structures. Suppose an En
lish speaker has
ever heard of ‘a
le
ancakes’ and runs across the term in a cookbook. He or she
will immediately recognize a
l
an
ancakes
and perceive the extremely close
imilarity of this to the schema 8.aa (= 6.aa), and the likelihood in any case that,
ince it consists of two food nouns following each other
it is a subcase of the well-
ntr
n
h
F
N-F
N
n
tr
ti
n 8
a
=6.ad
. The result will be a structure
uch as 6.a
, Similarly, if one wants to construct a new form to describe a curry dish
n which octopus is a ma
or in
redient, the easiest thin
to do is to use the words
ctopus an
in a construction (8.ak) sanctioned b
8.ad. Even in the absence of
ore s
ecific constructions octo
us
F
N
r F
N-curr
(which, if they exist,
are likely to be quite marginal), such sanction is enough.
This is how rule-governed linguistic creativity works under CG. The same
chematic structures that are extracted from well-entrenched (sub)cases may (though
hey need not) also be used as patterns for generating novel su
a
Th
an
ti
n
s central to both the understanding of already-established forms and the production
f new ones. So the model is not
eared onl
towards the formation of new words
nor onl
towards the anal
sis of the existin
word-stock; rather it accommodates
th
ith th
am
m
hani
m
The difference between rule-governed creativity and linguistic creativity in a
ore general sense is a matter of the strength and closeness of the sanction the
stablished system affords a novel usage. This is certainly stronger for
uillotines