11.2
Earlier attempts
at subgrouping
the Dravidian
languages
491
[± human], but ann¯ı or all¯a is only [−human], e.g.
atanu
1
cepp-in-di
2
all¯a
3
c¯es-
æ
-
.
du
4
[he speak-past-noml all do-past-3m-sg]
‘he
1
did
4
all
3
that has been said
2
(by/to him)’
If Andronov chose all¯a, it would have been a case of retaining a cognate (ell¯a in Ta.
Ma. Ka.), but he chose ant¯a and noted it as a case of loss of a cognate. Again, for
item 31 ‘foot’, he cited Telugu p¯adamu, a learned borrowing from Sanskrit, although
a
.
dugu, a cognate with Ta. Ma. Ka. a
.
di, is the one widely used in Modern Telugu. By his
lexicostatistic study he worked out the time distance between pairs of languages. The
closest sisters, Tamil–Malay¯a
.
lam with a retention rate 73 per cent of cognates, are said
to have been separated by 1,043 years, i.e. the tenth century AD, which is, in any case,
the kno
wn historical date. The greatest
time depth is between Telugu
and Brahui, with
16 per cent retention of cognates indicating a distance of 6,075 years or 4100 BC. What is
surprising is that every language is separated from Brahui by over 5,000 years including
its closest sisters Ku
.
rux (by 5,505 years) and Malto (5,874)! Ku
.
rux and Malto are shown
to be closer to Tamil (4,596 and 4,872 years, respectively) than to Brahui (Andronov
1964c: 184). The fact of the matter is that Brahui has retained only 15 per cent of native
lexical items and the influence of Balochi has been immense, despite its contact with
Balochi being only for 1,000
years (Elfenbein 1987: 219,
229). We still do not have a
measure of how fast borrowed words replace native items. The misleading time depth is
caused by loss of many cognates in Brahui because of heavy borrowing from Balochi and
Indo-Aryan. However, in terms of shared phonological and morphological innovations,
it could not have been separated for more than a thousand years or so from Ku
.
rux–Malto.
Further, the Brahui specialist, Elfenbein, says, ‘...the estimate by “glottochronological”
methods that Brahui separated from the rest ca. 3000 BC, is perhaps not to be taken too
seriously’ (1987: 229). This is enough for Andronov’s glottochronology.
There are two other lexicostatistical studies by Kameswari (1969) and Namboodiri
(1976) that I reviewe
d in 1980 (see Krishnamurti 1985/2001a: 256
–7). There is wide
variation in the dates of separation of individual languages by the two authors, which
I pointed out as evidence for the unreliability of the technique employed; for instance,
‘Tamil and Telugu diverged around 400 BC to AD 400 (Kameswari), 11th century BC
(Namboodiri)’ (Krishnamurti 1985/2001a: 256).
11.2.2 Other proposals
Krishnamurti (1969b/2001a: 114–17, 1985/2001a: 255–7) has surveyed the earlier views
on subgrouping and the reasons for revisions at each stage. In TVB (ch. 4), he proposed
three branches: South Dravidian (treated as South Dravidian I in this volume), Central