the words used to describe sensations are nothing
more than labels to represent sensations – no causal-
ity is inferred. It is risky to decide a priori what
word(s) subjects should use to describe a particular
sensation, or still greater risk that naive consumers
will understand them. Recall that about 30% of any
population of consumers cannot differentiate prod-
ucts at least at chance. In addition, subjects will not
be equally sensitive to all sensations. Some sensations
are complex, and in all likelihood subjects will want
to use more than a single word to represent that
experience. The fact that product changes based on
formulation, process, or both rarely yield a single
sensation means that, for example, a flavor change
would not be totally represented. To restrict subjects
to specific words assumes that the language is
unchanging, as are the meanings assigned to it, and
that all subjects will associate a specific word to a
specific sensation. The frame of reference for an attri-
bute is unique to each subject, and attributes requir-
ing a very different frame of reference are difficult for
inexperienced and/or nontechnical subjects to under-
stand. While chemical terminology is supposed to
have specific meaning to an expert, it is unlikely to
have the same meaning to a consumer, to a trained
subject, or to another expert.
0017 It should be noted that the use of technical termin-
ology extends training time and this appears to be
related to the complex nature of this terminology,
especially for subjects with no technical training.
The spectrum method provides the subjects with at-
tributes, as well as references and designated intensity
scores for those references, all of which are intended
to enhance the testing process. The use of standard or
previously specified attributes and references implies
that product variables will not produce unique sensa-
tions, that references themselves are not variable, and
forces subjects to limit the value of their perceptions
relative to what has been perceived in the past by
others. The training effort is extended over a period
of 3 months; this raises questions as to its responsive-
ness and overlooks the inherent variability in the
subjects (over time) and the references that are being
used (that also change over time). Regardless of
the source, a language that does not provide for
subject input is unlikely to yield uncomplicated sens-
ory responses. Subjects are influenced by the infor-
mation given to them, and are much less likely to
question it, because of its source. While it can be
argued that such approaches are merely intended to
help panel leaders and the subjects, the temptation is
very strong to use this approach rather than allowing
subjects to use their own terminology and, most
important, to allow subjects to express what they
perceive.
0018To the student of the history of psychology, descrip-
tive analysis can be considered as a form of introspec-
tion, a methodology used by the school of psychology
known as structuralism in its study of the human
experience. Structuralism required the use of highly
trained observers in a controlled situation verbalizing
their conscious experience (the method of introspec-
tion). Of particular interest to us is the use of the
method of introspection as an integral part of
the descriptive analysis process and, specifically, the
language development component.
0019However, the obvious difference is that products
are included (in descriptive analysis) so that the sub-
ject’s responses are perceptual and not conceptual.
Subjects are encouraged to use any words, provided
that they explain to the other members of the panel
what they mean – they define the meaning of each
word-sensation experience. The goal is for the panel,
by consensus, to associate a particular sensation with
a particular word or group of words. While each
subject begins with his or her own set of words, they
work as a group to come to agreement as to the
meaning of those words, i.e., the definitions or ex-
planations for each word-sensation experience, and
also when they (the sensations) occur. In addition,
they also develop a standard evaluation procedure.
All of these activities require time; in the QDA meth-
odology, there can be as many as five consecutive
daily sessions, each lasting about 90 min. This
amount of time is essential if the sensory language is
to be developed and understood, and the subjects are
capable of using it (and the scale) to differentiate the
products. These sessions help to identify the word-
attributes that could be misunderstood, and also
enable the subjects to practice scoring products and
discussing results, on an attribute-by-attribute basis.
Of course, all this effort cannot make subjects equally
sensitive to all attributes. In fact, subjects rarely, if
ever, achieve complete agreement for all attributes,
nor are subjects equally sensitive to all attributes, nor
should it be expected (if this did occur, one could rely
on the n of 1). The effectiveness of the training pro-
cess can be determined only after a test, after each
subject has scored products on a repeated trial basis
and the data have been analyzed. The idea that there
should be a specific number of attributes is, at best,
questionable, as is the issue of whether or not one has
the correct attributes. How does one know that all
the attributes have been developed or that they are
the right ones? The answer to the former is empirical
(and in part answered in much the same way as one
determines the number of angels that can occupy the
head of a pin). The answer to the latter is also empir-
ical, i.e., given a set of variables, do some attributes
exhibit systematic changes as a function of those
5156 SENSORY EVALUATION/Descriptive Analysis