ENGLISH
MORPHOPHONEMIC WRITING
117
then apply knowledge
of the
prefixes
and
suffixes.
For
nonnative speaking
students
whose linguistic competence develops
slowly
and
whose reading
vocabulary
is
often meager, direct instruction
in the
derivational mor-
phemes
of
English, although time-consuming,
may be
extremely
helpful,
especially
to
those
who
wish
to
pursue higher education
in an
English-
speaking
environment.
Do
ESL and EFL
readers
use
knowledge
of
English morphology
and
pro-
cessing
strategies
to
read unknown words?
The
main strategies that
ESL and
EFL
learners
can use in
word recognition
are
cognate recognition (Carroll,
1992),
context (Bensoussan
&
Laufer,
1984), graphemic similarity (Walker,
1983),
and
morphological processing.
The
cognate strategy
is
only available
to
ESL and EFL
readers
of
languages that
are
Germanic
or
Latin-derived.
The use of
context
is
only available
if
there
is
sufficient
surrounding informa-
tion
and it can be
utilized
by the
reader. Graphemic similarity
is of
limited
use
in
English. Osburne
and
Mulling (1998),
in
their survey
of
this literature,
found
that students prefer these strategies
and
rarely rely
on a
morphologi-
cal
strategy
to
help them
identify
unknown words.
In a new
study Osburne
and
Mulling (2001) found that many Spanish speaking
ESL
students could
use
a
morphological strategy
if
necessary,
but
they preferred
not to,
presum-
ably
because
of the
cognitive load that morphological processing entails.
Cognitive
load refers
to the
amount
of
mental work involved
in a
task—the
more work
there
is, the
more
reluctant
the
reader
is to do it.
There
are a
number
of
reasons that might account
for the
large cognitive
load
involved
in
processing English morphology. First, processing deri-
vational
morphology involves disassembling
the
word into component mor-
phemes (which could
be
ambiguous), matching them with sound representa-
tions
(which
are
opaque,
as
discussed earlier), accessing them
in the
mental
lexicon
and
semantic memory (where they might
not
occur),
and
reassem-
bling
the
pieces into
the
whole word.
ESL and EFL
students
may not
have
the
knowledge
base
or
processing strategies
to do
that,
or
their
processing strate-
gies
might
not
work with
automaticity.
A
further contributing factor might
be
that
the
students'
own
knowledge
of
their
LI
morphology
and the
processing
strategies they have already developed
may
interfere with processing English
morphology.
MORPHOLOGY
IN
OTHER LANGUAGES
Comrie
(1981)
offered
a
useful
way to
discuss morphology
in the
world's
languages
by
introducing
the
concept
of two
morphological dimensions.
One
dimension concerns
the
number
of
morphemes
per
word
and the
other dimension concerns
the
extent
to
which
the
morphemes within
a
word
can be
segmented
or
separated from each
other.
Languages
can be
placed
on the
axes
of two
continua,
as
shown
in
Fig. 8.2.
Languages
at
point
A are
called
isolating;
normally, each word
is
made
up of one
morpheme.
For
example, although Chinese
has
some com-