APPROACHES
TO
PHONICS
93
There
is
much
we can say
about phonic generalizations,
but the
long
and
the
short
of it is
that
the low or
unpredictable utility
of
many
of
them made
teachers
feel
that they were
not
useful
to
teach
and
practice. Sometimes
al-
though
the
generalizations were often true (when
a
word begins
with
kn,
the
k is
silent),
it
seemed
a
waste
of
class time
to
explain
it and
then
do a
worksheet
on
that
one
pattern.
Many
teachers were
eager
to
turn
away
from
this
type
of
phonics instruction
and
embrace whole language methods that
often
assumed that beginning readers would just learn phonic generaliza-
tions
on
their
own
through exposure
to
print (Weaver,
1994).
It is
true that
readers
do
unconsciously acquire knowledge
of
these phonic generaliza-
tions
through exposure
to
print,
but
they
are not in the
form
of
overt rules.
Rather,
they
form
the
unconscious probabilistic
and
context-dependent
knowledge
and
processing strategies
we saw in the
last chapter.
From
our
current perspective
in ESL and
EFL,
we can see
that phonic
generalizations
and the
deductive synthetic phonics instruction that accom-
panied them
fall
into
the
category
of
learning about
the
language rather
than
acquiring
the use of the
language.
We
think
it
commonplace
now
that
learning
a
grammar rule doesn't necessary imply that
the
learner
will
be
able
to
apply
the
rule
in
speaking
or
writing. Likewise, learning
the
phonic
generalizations such
as
those previously mentioned doesn't lead
to
automaticity;
so
those teachers
who
found these phonic generalizations
te-
dious
and
unhelpful
were probably right. When
the
teaching
of
phonic gen-
eralizations
was
largely discarded, however,
an
important thread
of
reading
instruction
was
also lost
for
some teachers.
In
their eagerness
not to
teach
phonic generalizations, some teachers stopped explicit phonics instruction
altogether.
A
similar thing
has
happened
with
the
blending strategy
which
used
to
be
quite commonly taught
in
English
LI
reading instruction.
Teachers
saw
that
trying
to
figure
out the
pronunciation
of a
graph
in
isolation
led to
many
errors
and
problems. Some children would
say the
letter name
in-
stead
of the
sound;
siy ey tiy for cat
will
never "blend" into
its
proper
pro-
nunciation.
Some children, although they could assign
a
sound
and not a
letter name, chose
the
wrong sound
to
assign
and
they also encountered
problems when trying
to
blend
the
sounds
together
to
figure
out the
word.
For
many teachers, blending also went
out the
window
as
they began
to
pre-
fer
whole language methods.
Although
some phonics instruction
in the
past
was
rule-based
and
syn-
thetic, another phonics instructional method, called
the
Linguistic Method,
was
based
on
learning
key
spelling patterns like -at, bat, cat, sat, fat, etc.
(Tierney
&
Readence, 2000) Although this
has
turned
out to be a
good
method
of
teaching reading
in
English,
at the
time
the
method
was in
vogue
the
materials were based largely around meaningless nonwords
or
silly
sto-
ries
with
sentences like "Dan
can fan
Nan."
Teachers
quite rightly criticized
this
phonics method because
it did not
provide early
readers
with
much
mo-
tivation
to
read.
It was
dull
and
unrealistic.
The
purpose
of
these stories
was