78
CHAPTER
6
the
compound grapheme occurs
after
lax
vowels,
before certain
suffixes,
and
so
on. The
English writing system
is
therefore more complex
for the
writing
decision-making
system
than
it is for the
reading decision-making system,
which
may
account
for the
impression
of
chaos surrounding
the
system.
Spelling rules have some similarities
with
reading rules.
For one
thing,
they
draw
on the
same linguistic knowledge that readers have
in
their heads
about
graphemes
and
phonemes. Reading rules
and
spelling rules
are of-
ten
taught
at the
same time. However, reading rules
and
spelling rules
are
fundamentally
different
in
their
functions
and
application.
The
correspon-
dence that goes
from
grapheme
to
phoneme
is far
more predictable,
be-
cause,
for the
most part, there
are
fewer
phonemes than potential
graphemes associated
with
them.
People also think that
the
English writing system
is
irregular because
of
their expectations
of
what
an
alphabetic writing system should
be.
Many
people have
the
idea that
a
perfect writing
system
would have
a
certain
number
of
symbols,
26
say, with
one
symbol
for
each sound
and one
sound
for
each symbol. English writing
is not
like that. First,
we
have more pho-
nemes
in our
language than
we
have alphabet letters.
And
second,
we
have
more graphemes than alphabet letters too.
A
radical solution would
be to
double
the
number
of
alphabet letters, assign
one
ambiguously
to
each pho-
neme,
and
begin writing
in
this
new
way. Indeed, some naive reformers
have
advocated this
and
other
similar solutions. However, reforming
our
spelling
has
proven
to be as
resistant
to
change
as the
U.S. conversion
to the
metric
system,
so
such
a
radical spelling reform
is
highly unlikely. Although
the
spelling
of
some words could benefit
from
some "pruning,"
the
system
itself
works
well
enough.
To see the
pattern
in
English spelling,
we
must
rid
ourselves
of the ex-
pectation that alphabet symbols must have
a
one-to-one
correspondence
to
phonemes
for
that alphabetic writing
to be
regular
and
consistent. Instead,
let's
think about
a
complex system
in
which,
first of
all, there
are
more
graphemes than alphabet letters. Both
b and bb are
graphemes. Second,
most
consonant graphemes (except
c, g, and gh) are
read unambiguously
because they
do
correspond
to one
phoneme
of
English. Sometimes
the
phonemes
correspond
to
more than
one
grapheme,
but
that
is not the
problem
for
reading
as it is for
writing. When
the
aforementioned charts
are
examined
in
this
new
light, regularity
and
consistency
are
evident. Reg-
ular
and
consistent patterns
of
correspondences between graphemes
and
phonemes, even
if
they
are
complex, make
it
easy
for the
reading processor
to
make decisions about assigning
a
phoneme
to a
grapheme
in
reading.
Once
the
orthographic processor
is
trained (through experience, practice,
and
direct instruction)
to the
point
of
automaticity
to
recognize these
graphemes,
it is not
hard
for it to
associate
the
correct pronunciation
with
them
because they
are
highly
predictable.
We
can say
that these tables contain
raw
probabilities that
a
single
grapheme
will
be
pronounced
a
certain way. However, knowledge
and
per-