effective strategy for fat reduction is to decrease the
preference for high levels of dietary fat. The effects of
two different reduced-fat diets have been studied: one
allow the use of fat replacers and one which did not,
and the effects on the preference for a limited range of
high-fat foods were noted. This study found that the
group which was given low-fat foods without fat
replacers (no fatty sensory qualities) showed a de-
crease in the preferred level of fat in foods, whereas
the group using fat replacers showed no such shift. It
was concluded that the preference for fat in foods is
governed more by exposure to fatty flavors than by
the actual level of fat in foods. It was further sug-
gested that the preference for fat can be lowered, and
that the best strategy for lowering fat preference is,
therefore, to avoid foods with fat replacers. Because
of methodological limitations, however, firm conclu-
sions cannot be drawn from this research. First, he-
donic measures were obtained on only four foods.
Since fat imparts so many different sensory properties
to foods, it appears unlikely that changes in prefer-
ences for fat in one type of food will generalize to
other types of foods. Second, because subjects con-
sumed products at home, there was little control over
the experimental setting. Finally, data were obtained
from self-reported diet records, with scant checks for
compliance.
0020 A recent study measured participants’ sensory
ratings to foods with various levels of fat while they
were consuming diets that varied in overall fat con-
tent: 37, 30, and 26% energy from fat. Participants
rated their sensitivity to and liking for fat in several
foods at baseline, after each 8-week diet period, and
at the end of the entire study. They found no differ-
ences in liking of foods as a function of fat level of the
overall diet. Thus, exposure to a low-fat diet for an
extended period did not diminish fat preference, and
exposure to a higher-fat diet did not increase fat
preference. In this study, however, the low-fat diets
did not use fat-replaced foods exclusively. Thus, al-
though the hypothesis that exposure to fat-replaced
foods may maintain a fat preference may have merit,
further research is needed to investigate this issue
fully.
0021 Energy density Metabolism of dietary fat yields ap-
proximately 9 kcal g, more than twice the yield of
4 kcal g for carbohydrate or protein. There is evidence
that people tend to eat a consistent weight of food.
Given this, the relatively high energy density of fat
would be an important factor in its overconsumption.
For example, 50 g of potato chips (which typically
derive 60% of their energy from fat) provides
269 kcal of energy, while an equal weight of pretzels
(which typically derive about 8% of their energy from
fat or less) provides only 187 kcal. This effect was
tested experimentally in a serving of a milk-based
preload that had varying amounts of water added to
change the volume (300, 450, and 600 ml), while
macronutrient and energy content and palatability
were held constant. The resulting drinks differed solely
in the density of the provided nutrients and energy.
The results showed that the energy density of these
milk-drink preloads affected energy intake at lunch
30 min later, such that intake was significantly lower
(by 18%) after the high-volume, low-energy-density
drink than after the low-volume, high-energy-density
drink. Further, this energy deficit was not compen-
sated for at the dinner meal. Thus, consuming more
energy-dense, higher-fat foods may easily lead to
higher daily energy intakes than eating foods that
are low in fat and less energy-dense.
0022Fat and satiety It has been proposed that fat may be
overeaten because it does not satisfy hunger as well as
other macronutrients. Ingested fat may differ from
ingested carbohydrate and protein in how it affects
such factors as stomach distention, stomach empty-
ing, nutrient absorption, hormonal release, and/or
oxidation of nutrients. Indeed, fat and carbohydrate
have very different postingestive dispositions. In ex-
periments that measured postabsorptive metabolism,
dietary fat was shown to be metabolized much more
slowly than carbohydrate or protein. Carbohydrate
ingestion produces rapid rises in blood glucose, while
fat ingestion often depresses blood glucose. The ‘glu-
costatic theory,’ which suggests that the sensation of
hunger is maintained until blood glucose levels reach
adequate levels, would posit that carbohydrates pro-
duce more rapid satiety than fats. Opponents of the
glucostatic theory argue that there are other, more
important factors influencing satiety, including the
release of ‘satiety hormones.’
0023One such putative satiety hormone is cholecysto-
kinin (CCK). CCK is particularly released when
amino acids and/or some types of fat enter the small
intestine. Thus, the ingestion of fat could lead to early
satiety despite having less of an effect on blood
glucose than carbohydrates. Because of these
differing physiological effects, it is difficult to make
any definitive statements regarding the satiety value
of fat versus carbohydrate. However, data from con-
trolled-laboratory feeding studies suggest that, for
some people, carbohydrate may be more satiating.
One group reviewed various studies that utilized a
preloading model (giving fixed amounts of macronu-
trients and assessing the effects on subsequent satiety/
hunger and food intake) to detect any differences in
the satiety value of fat versus carbohydrate. They
concluded that carbohydrate seems to have greater
2250 FAT SUBSTITUTES/Use of Fat-replaced Foods in Reducing Fat and Energy Intake