Speakers' Responses to Requests for Repetition 277
relations to the following utterance and thus can be used to enhance the process-
ing of the post-RR utterance. Our next step in working with this data will be to
incorporate these relations in a statistical language model for speech recognition,
exploiting these relationships to improve performance.
On the other hand, very few of these linguistic results were in any way af-
fected by the media through which the conversation took place. An examination
of media use suggests that, since users are largely unfamiliar with non-speech
options for (real- time) communication, their use of these options is dependent
upon their own, individual, judgments rather than upon any generalized social
conventions. The wide variety of ways of using non-speech media observed in the
course of the experiment do not reveal any particular recurring, consistent pat-
tern that could be exploited in enhancing the performance of automatic language
processing systems.
We suggested that the results reported here have implications for the nature
of effective constraints for a system processing spontaneous speech. Speakers
should be encouraged to reduce the linguistic aspects of their utterances in ways
in which they are already inclined to do so: by eliminating unnecessary phrases
from their syntactic structures, reducing lexical variability and disfluencies, and
slowing down their speech. Instructions to speak simply, clearly and slowly would
make explicit the strategies that speakers employ spontaneously when faced with
a difficult communication situation.
The next step, then, is to provide some sort of constraint upon media use.
This constraint could be imposed in one of two ways, either by providing explicit
instructions or by encouraging pre-existing 'intuitive' strategies. Recall that, in
this experiment, the primary phrase used by the Wizard to indicate lack of un-
derstanding was
"please repeat."
For certain types of language processing break-
down, the 'machine' might be given the option to request the client explicitly
to
"please type"
or
"please draw."
Pre-conversation instructions which contain
even more specific injunctions, say, to type
all
hotel reservation information or
to draw a circle on the map to indicate location, could also be included.
Ultimately, however, we would hope that constraints on media use will paral-
lel those on language use. That is, as more and more people become experienced
in the use of multimedia systems, it will be possible to draw on their intuitive,
media-related
responses to communication difficulties just as we propose to draw
on the intuitive
linguistic
responses of the subjects in this experiment. One very
recently completed experiment in EMMI involved frequent users of multime-
dia systems, whose experience has supplied them with some internal model for
efficient and effective use of non-speech options. Preliminary results indicate
some ways in which these users differ from 'naive' users: experienced users are
much more likely to repeat their utterances exactly, instead of changing them
for clarification; they also appear to use typing especially as a means to clarify
utterances, to a greater extent than the subjects reported above. By studying
how these users respond to RR's in this way, it will be possible to design me-
dia systems that encourage 'natural' media-related responses to communication