2.18 Kams´a 153
Table 2.25 Kams´a personal reference markers
(after Mel´endez Lozano 2000c)
1 pers. sing. c(i)-, s(
)-, i-
1 and 3 pers. dual bo-, b
-, bn-
1 pers. plur. exclusive ϕc(i)-, ϕs(
)-
1 pers. plur. inclusive and 3 pers. plur. mo-, m(
)-
2 pers. sing. ko-, k(
)-
2 pers. dual ˇs
.
o-, ˇs
.
-, ˇs
.
n-
2 pers. plur. ˇs
.
mo-, ˇs
.
m(
)-
A characteristic feature of the Kams´a language are its consonant clusters. Up to three
consonants (rarely even four) can be found in medial and initial position, e.g. in ˇsknen´a
‘wooden plate’, x
ϕcbeˇc
.
´ana ‘to carry a child’,
ŋ
goϕˇsn´a ‘green’ (Howard 1979: 89). The
place of the accent is contrastive. It can be either on the last or on the penultimate vowel
of a word.
Typologically, Kams´a differs from any other language in the area by its abundant use of
prefixes. Nevertheless, prefixation is not the only morphological device of the language.
Case and number with nouns, as well as several verbal categories, are indicated by means
of suffixes. A set of eighteen classifiers based on shape (e.g. -b´e for round objects, -ˇc
.
e
for oval objects, -ϕxa for rigid cylindrical objects, -xa for flexible cylindrical objects, -ˇs
.
´a
for hairy and/or composed elements, -ye for liquids) can be suffixed to demonstrative
pronouns, numerals and adjectives (cf. Mel´endez Lozano 2000c: 136). They also occur
as suffixes with nouns; e.g. in flor-ˇs
.
´a ‘flower’, from Spanish flor, and in eskardon´a-ˇc
.
e ‘an
insect’ (Howard 1979: 88), presumably from a local Spanish word escardona (compare
escardar ‘to weed’).
Howard (1977a) distinguishes eight distinct slots of affixes that can precede the verbal
root in narrative discourse. The functions of the prefixes occurring in these slots belong to
the domain of narrative type (historical versus legendary event), evidentiality (presence
and/or participation of the speaker in the event), personal reference, movement, tense
and aspect. Mood (imperative, contrary-to-fact, potential), negation and interrogation
are also expressed by means of prefixes. Mel´endez Lozano (2000c) lists nine modal
prefixes (including negative and evidential) and ten aspectual prefixes. He reports the
personal reference markers used for identification of the subject (based on Juajibioy
Chindoy and Wheeler 1973), as indicated in table 2.25.
His enumeration does not contain any markers referring to a third person singular.
The non-singular third-person markers coincide with non-singular first-person markers.
Howard (1977a: 7) gives o- for third-person-singular subject. However, many third-
person-singular subject forms occurring in her examples seem to lack this prefix,