12 PA
L
TEKA
ER
ultilevel model of linguistic sign (1983, and Horecký et al., 1989). At the same
ime, it responds to the one-sided formalism of the mainstream generative word-
f
rmati
n
The general linguistic background is that of the functional-structural
approach of the Prague School of Linguistics. Therefore, the form-meaning unit
.e., the bilateral nature of morphemes
s regarded as the fundamental principle.
3.3.1 Word-formation as an independent componen
The basic scope and principles of word
f
rmati
n
an
fin
a
f
ll
(1) Word-formation deals with productive and rule-
overned patterns
word-formation t
pes and rules, and morpholo
ical t
pes) used to
generate motivated naming unit
in response to the specific naming
eeds of a
articular s
eech co
munity by making use of word-
formation bases of bilateral naming units and affixes stored in the
Lexical Component.
The individual aspects of this definition are discussed below. The co
nitive
nomasiolo
ical theor
identifies word-formation as an
nde
endent com
onen
inguistics, as illustrated in Figure 1. The scheme re
resents a crucial triad o
between extra-linguistic reality (object to be named), a speech community
represented by a ‘coiner’), and the wo
d-formation component, thus emphasising
the fact, ignored by the vast ma
ority of the mainstream word-formation theories,
that each act of naming responds
o a very real and specific
aming demand
n th
part of a member (members) of
peech communit
The notion of speech communit
hould not be taken absolutel
, i.e., there is hardl
an
word-formation process
which responds to a namin
demand of all the speakers of a particular lan
ua
e.
Rather, such a demand is closely connected with a limited number of ‘first-contact’
users; a coinage may or may not subsequently find a wider use.
he above-mentioned triad reflects the following principles:
a) It lays emphasis on the
r
f language users in the process of giving
ames to objects instead of presenting word-formation as an impersonal syste
f rules detached from the objects named and from language users.
b) The naming act is not a purely linguistic act. Naming units do not come into
existence in isolation from factors
uch as human knowledge, human cognitive
abilities, experiences, discoveries of new things, processes, and qualities, human
ma
ination, etc. This position is in a
r
an
ith K
h’
i
a that th
nomasiolo
ical viewpoint is closer to that of the speaker as a lin
uistic
nnovator than the semasiolo
ical viewpoint (2001: 17). An ob
ect to be named
An important and most valuable e
eption to this formalism is Beard’s Lexeme-Morpheme Base
orpholog
1995
cf. cha
ter... in thi
volume) which is, in effect, a variant of an onomasiological
approach to word-formation.
This term was first introduced by Mathesius (1975
In my approach, it sub
it
t
f
r th
t
rm
lik
ord
lexem
etc.
because of their inconsistent u
and various connotations in linguistic
iterature. “Naming unit” refers here to a compl
x unit generated by the Word-Formation Component.