PA
L
TEKA
ER
2. METHODS OF ONOMASIOLOGICAL RESEARCH
Onomasiology is not restricted to word-formation. Actually, its scope has been
uch broader from its ince
tion,
covering the field of lexicology. As defined by B.
Quadri, onomasiolo
studies the wa
s of lan
ua
es and their dialects in expressin
a particular concept. The point of departure for an onomasiolo
ical approach is
always a concept (1952: 1). Its tasks and objectives were identified by E. Tappolet
1895: 4) as answering a series of questions: How does a language at a particula
ime and at a
articular
lace ex
ress a c
nce
t? Does it take over an ex
ression
from an earlier period or is the original
xpression replaced by a new one? In the
former case, are the form and meaning identi
al with the original ones? In the latte
ase, in what way and by which means is the new expression formed? And the final
uestion is the ‘Wh
’? question: What was the reason for the chan
e in expressin
ne and the same concept? And, is it actuall
still the same concept?
There are two basic divisions in onomasiolo
ical research. The first dichotom
n
rn
th
ynchrony vs. diachrony
osition, the second bears on the em
irical
r
esearch. The individual a
roaches
ay also be combined. Empirical
nomasiology studies the different ways of expressing (empirical aspect) a given
oncept in various languages (synchronic aspect) and/or the etymology of these
expressions and their changes over time
diachronic aspect). The diachronic
empirical method has been the dominant research method, even if – it should be
oted – the ma
or part of works written within this framework fall within the scope
f lexical semantics rather than word-formation. Nevertheless
Blank’s definition of
he scope of diachronic cognitive onoma
ology accommodates both semantic and
ord-formation
ers
ectives:
It investigates the main strategies that e
st in a language sample for conceptualizing
and verbalizing a given concept and tri
to explain them against a cognitive
background in terms of salient perceptions, p
minency, convincing similarities, etc. It
asks for the source concepts that seem to b
universally recurrent, lays bare associative
elations between source and target concepts and describes the lexical processes used by
he speakers... This theo
ti
al f
n
ati
n al
all
th
escription and explanation of
han
es towards a co
nitivel
more promine
strate
and of
eor
anizations o
onceptual structures (2001: 21-22).
There are a
reat number of empirical
nomasiolo
ical studie
For illustration
linei (1995), anal
zes different names for the concept of
GLASSES an
emonstrates different motivations underl
in
the namin
of this concept in various
anguages, ranging from the semantic shift based on the associative principle (Engl.
lasses), throu
h coinin
a new word based on the conti
uit
It.
o
imilarity relation (Fr.
) to borrowing from French
r
r
er.
r
As noted b
B. Quadri (1952: vii), while the term ‘onomasiolo
’ was introduced b
Adolf Zaune
1902), the first ‘onomasiological’ wor
Romanische Wortschöpfung
1875) by Friedrich Diez was
published as early as 1875.
As specified below, contiguity i
a conceptual, extralinguistic relationship primarily characteristic of
eton
m
. In Koch’s approach (1999b: 146ff), conti
uit
is the relation that exists between
prototypical, salient elements of a frame or betwee
the frame as a whole and its elements. One more
exam
le from Koch
1999b
will illustrate th
point. The metonymical shift of Engl.
r ‘
nt
r’ t