Назад
Organizational Decisionmaking by Terrorist Groups 235
of greatest importance. e choices of groups with complex internal
dynamics or competing internal factions may be dominated by how
they will play” for internal constituencies. Some groups will be opti-
mizers, seeking to make the best choices possible given their circum-
stances; others will satisfice, seeking what is good enoughrather than
optimal. Groups will also have different preferences and tolerances for
such factors as operational risk or incomplete information. Although a
given level of information or risk may reach an acceptable threshold for
one organization and it will decide to act, another organization might
instead decide to gather more intelligence or defer a decision until the
risks of acting can be reduced.
Implications for Strategy and Policy
In considering strategies for counterterrorism aimed at the organiza-
tional level, the decisionmaking processes of terrorist groups are one
potential target for action. In general, if options are unavailable to take
on terrorist groups directly, action to complicate or shape their deci-
sionmaking in ways that make it more difficult for them to plan and
stage violent actions can be an alternative. To the extent that the factors
shaping a particular groups decision processes can be identified and
understood, defensive measures can also be better crafted to frustrate
their operations or guide group choices in ways that are favorable for
defense and security organizations. Clear models, based on available
social-science understandings of terrorist organizational behaviors, can
help to guide such policy design.
In considering terrorist group decisionmaking, and opportuni-
ties for counterterrorism action, there is an existing basis for think-
ing: Efforts at deterrence and influence are, at their most basic,
efforts to shape the choices that groups make about the things they
do and the ways they attempt to do them. From this perspective,
security measures seek to shape group risk tolerance, as do clas-
sic” attempts at deterrence through threats of punishment or retali-
ation for specific acts; information operations telegraphing how use
of unconventional weapons would be viewed negatively in a groups
236 Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces Together
Figure 6.1
Factors Shaping Terrorist Group Decisionmaking
RAND MG849-6.1
Likelihood of
decision to act
Scope of positive reaction
action will produce in
relevant population
Legitimacy and
acceptability of
target, means,
action, etc.
Consistency
with group
ideology
Level of
pressure to act
imposed by
competing
groups
Level of
pressure to act
imposed by
external events
or demands
Consistency with
group goals/
interests
Consistency
with
preferences of
state sponsors
Level of
alignment of
action with
any external
influences
Consistency with
preferences of strategic
or operational
influences in networks
or movements
Consistency
with
preferences of
cooperating
groups
Permissiveness
of group
success criteria
Defenses at
desired
targets
Group OPSEC
effectiveness
Scope of group
dynamics that
skew preferences
(e.g., groupthink)
Effect of past
behavior bias on
future preferences
Effects of
clandestinity on
group activities
Effectiveness
of state CT
activities
Group
capability
level
Level of
member risk
tolerance
Biasing effect
of recent
experience
(positive and
negative)
Group risk
tolerance
Group effects
on risk
decisions
Availability
of resources
from others
Amount of time
for planning,
prep and
implementation
Amount of
situational
awareness
information
available
to group
Amount of
technical
knowledge
available
to group
Access to
external
knowledge
sources
Insulation of group
from pressure
(e.g., safe haven)
Amount of money
available to group
Amount of
technology available
to group
(e.g., weapons)
Number of
appropriate
people in
group
Ability to
gather new
information
Breadth and
depth of
internal
“knowledge
stocks”
Legitimacy and
acceptability of
target, means,
action, etc.
Perceived
intensity of
need to act to
preserve group
cohesion
Level of “bias
to action”
within group
Intensity of
idiosyncratic
preferences
toward
particular
activity
Intensity of
idiosyncratic
preferences among
leadership
Intensity of
idiosyncratic
preferences among
membership
Forestalling a negative
event an included case of
a perceived positive
outcome?
Group perceptions
of other elements
that shape public
support for
terrorist activity
t5BSHFUFEHPWFSONFOU
t(FOFSBMQPQVMBUJPO
t4VQQPSUQPQVMBUJPO
Decisionmaker beliefs about:
and and
and
or or
(+/–)
(+/–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
Amount action will
advance group
strategy or interests
Scope of positive reaction
action will generate
inside group
Amount and
quality of group
communications
capability (to get
information to
decisionmaker)
Acceptability of
risks associated
with action
Level of resources
group is willing to
commit to action
Perceived sufficiency
of information to
make decision
Level of
“knowledge
threshold”
demanded
for decision
(+)
(–)
Organizational Decisionmaking by Terrorist Groups 237
Figure 6.1
Factors Shaping Terrorist Group Decisionmaking
RAND MG849-6.1
Likelihood of
decision to act
Scope of positive reaction
action will produce in
relevant population
Legitimacy and
acceptability of
target, means,
action, etc.
Consistency
with group
ideology
Level of
pressure to act
imposed by
competing
groups
Level of
pressure to act
imposed by
external events
or demands
Consistency with
group goals/
interests
Consistency
with
preferences of
state sponsors
Level of
alignment of
action with
any external
influences
Consistency with
preferences of strategic
or operational
influences in networks
or movements
Consistency
with
preferences of
cooperating
groups
Permissiveness
of group
success criteria
Defenses at
desired
targets
Group OPSEC
effectiveness
Scope of group
dynamics that
skew preferences
(e.g., groupthink)
Effect of past
behavior bias on
future preferences
Effects of
clandestinity on
group activities
Effectiveness
of state CT
activities
Group
capability
level
Level of
member risk
tolerance
Biasing effect
of recent
experience
(positive and
negative)
Group risk
tolerance
Group effects
on risk
decisions
Availability
of resources
from others
Amount of time
for planning,
prep and
implementation
Amount of
situational
awareness
information
available
to group
Amount of
technical
knowledge
available
to group
Access to
external
knowledge
sources
Insulation of group
from pressure
(e.g., safe haven)
Amount of money
available to group
Amount of
technology available
to group
(e.g., weapons)
Number of
appropriate
people in
group
Ability to
gather new
information
Breadth and
depth of
internal
“knowledge
stocks”
Legitimacy and
acceptability of
target, means,
action, etc.
Perceived
intensity of
need to act to
preserve group
cohesion
Level of “bias
to action”
within group
Intensity of
idiosyncratic
preferences
toward
particular
activity
Intensity of
idiosyncratic
preferences among
leadership
Intensity of
idiosyncratic
preferences among
membership
Forestalling a negative
event an included case of
a perceived positive
outcome?
Group perceptions
of other elements
that shape public
support for
terrorist activity
t5BSHFUFEHPWFSONFOU
t(FOFSBMQPQVMBUJPO
t4VQQPSUQPQVMBUJPO
Decisionmaker beliefs about:
and and
and
or or
(+/–)
(+/–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(–)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
(+)
Amount action will
advance group
strategy or interests
Scope of positive reaction
action will generate
inside group
Amount and
quality of group
communications
capability (to get
information to
decisionmaker)
Acceptability of
risks associated
with action
Level of resources
group is willing to
commit to action
Perceived sufficiency
of information to
make decision
Level of
“knowledge
threshold”
demanded
for decision
(+)
(–)
238 Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces Together
sympathizer community or by the international community seek to
shape a variety of different elements of decisionmaking and also the
apparent value of pursing them; diplomatic efforts to end state spon-
sorship seek to constrain group resources and reduce the chance that
the group will be willing to devote enough people and technology to
operations of concern; and so on.
72
Drawing on current social-science literature and other available
data on group decisionmaking, the model represented in Figure 6.1
captures the range of elements that shape group choices, from what
those choices are intended to accomplish to the risk and information
thresholds a group may impose on its on decisions. In framing these
factors, some elements go back through decades of terrorism research
and are supported by broad bodies of knowledge in related fields. For
example, the inuence of organizational dynamics on decisionmak-
ing has been recognized for many years, has been examined in a vari-
ety of terrorist organizations, and—given the much broader interest
in how all groups make choices—has been examined in a variety of
other types of organizations as well. Others factors are more provi-
sional, being based on smaller amounts of data or less direct study
of the behavior in terrorist groups. An example of this latter class is
questions about the effect of organizational risk tolerance on group
decisionmaking. Although there are enough data to make a case that
it is an important factor that needs to be considered and included, it
has not itself been systematically studied. As a result, in some cases,
concepts are framed just as they are in literature sources and my model
summarizes those concepts in a uniform structure; in others, I have
crafted categories to bring together separate (and sometimes still thin)
strands of thought that have not yet been fully explored or placed into
a useful framework.
When considering application of this model to guide thinking
about strategy and policy, some caveats are appropriate. First, for sim-
plicity the model is framed as a group considering a single choice, with
the factors shaping the likelihood of making a decision. In reality, deci-
sionmakers generally consider multiple options and make comparisons
among them—even if only to compare the consequences of an action
against the consequences of taking no action at all. As a result, the
Organizational Decisionmaking by Terrorist Groups 239
additional complication of making those comparisons among options,
and the effect of which options groups choose to weigh against each
other, is a factor that I have not considered explicitly.
Second, although understanding the factors that shape group
choices can make it possible to make more informed projections about
what groups may or may not do, real limits constrain the ability to
do so. ere are limits on the intelligence that is available about inter-
nal group deliberations. As a result, just as the terrorist group deci-
sionmaker will invariably have limits on available data, so too will the
policymaker or operator in counterterrorism.
ird, even a systematic and deliberative terrorist group that makes
the best decision it can at a given time can simply be wrong, meaning
that even with an excellent model that captures and integrates all avail-
able information on how choices are made, it may still not be possible
to predict the groups future behavior. To be rational does not mean
to be perfect. A variety of elements can affect the quality of a group’s
choices, including divergence between what the group thinks and what
truly is(for example, a group’s assumptions about how its actions will
be interpreted by their target audiences). A group can similarly have
misperceptions about its own levels of skills or knowledge that can
lead to miscalculation and error (Drake, 1998). Incorrect knowledge,
bad situational awareness, too little information, or too much data to
assess in the time available to the decisionmakers can similarly degrade
the quality of an organizations choices.
73
e situation the group faces
may also simply change, making choices that might have been benefi-
cial in one environment detrimental under new circumstances. As with
all groups, terrorist organizations have bounds on their understanding
and knowledge that make it difficult to project their own actions into
the future and understand their consequences with certainty.
Fourth, although many analyses approach terrorist groups as
single decisionmakers,the reality is that they (and all organizations)
are made up of individuals whose interests may or may not be entirely
congruent with those of the group as a whole. Individual actors “look-
ing out for themselves” can therefore inject different sets of preferences
into a decision process. Some authors have framed this as a principal-
agent problem given that central terrorist actors have difficulty con-
240 Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces Together
trolling what other members do, thus opening the opportunity for
self interested behavior (Chai, 1993; Shapiro, 2005a). Such individual
behaviors also arise as a result of rivalry within groups and contests for
power or resources.
74
Finally, although a group may make “the right choice” to advance
its interests in a specific situation, it is not certain that it will actually be
able to implement its decisions. Some of the same factors that were dis-
cussed above—such as command and control issues,
75
organizational
structure constraints, security concerns limiting communication, and
the bringing together of resourcescan also get in the way of a group’s
ability to translate thought into action.
As a result, even armed with the best that social-science research
currently has to offer, the task of understanding terrorist group deci-
sionmaking and anticipating group behavior must be approached with
humility. In doing so, the analyst (and a decisionmaker relying on the
analysis) must know something of the nature of the group itself. Exam-
ination must focus on the appropriate decisionmaking units within
an organization, where the results of social-science research on group
behavior are applicable, and should not assume that identical processes
apply in groups as vastly different as individual terrorist cells and loosely
coupled collections of individuals linked only by a common ideology.
Although both the interplay of ideas and the deliberative processes that
occur at all points on the spectrum of terrorist organizational behavior
are important, variations in the processes in different types of organiza-
tions must be understood and taken into account.
e fact that decisionmaking and the factors that shape it are
inherently idiosyncratic in individual terrorist groups means that gen-
eral models that can predict “terrorist behavior” will be elusive at best.
However, at the same time, even if models may not be predictive,
laying out the range of factors that shape terrorist group decisions can
still aid in understanding how groups of interest might behave and can
guide counterterrorist thinking. In examining a specific group and the
factors that are most important given the available information on its
leadership, membership, and environment, such models can help to
assess the possible effects of different strategies for deterrence or influ-
Organizational Decisionmaking by Terrorist Groups 241
ence and to prioritize those likely to be more or less likely to be effec-
tive against it.
Bibliography
Ahmed, Fazal, Terrorism as a Rational Tactic: An International Study, Ann Arbor,
Mich.: University of Michigan, 1998.
Al-Zawahiri, Ayman, letter to Abu Mus’ab al-Zarqawi, July, 9, 2005. As of January
5, 2008:
http://www.globalsecurity.org/security/library/report/2005/zawahiri-zarqawi-
letter_9jul2005.htm
Anderton, Charles, and John Carter, “On Rational Choice eory and the Study
of Terrorism,Defence and Peace Economics, Vol. 16, No. 4, 2005, pp. 275–282.
Arquilla, John, and David F. Ronfeldt, “e Advent of Netwar: Analytic
Background,Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, Vol. 22, No. 3, 1999, pp. 193–206.
———, eds., Networks and Netwars: e Future of Terror, Crime, and Militancy,
Santa Monica, Calif.: RAND Corporation, 2001. As of January 5, 2009:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/monograph_reports/MR1382/
Atran, Scott, and Marc Sageman, “Connecting the Dots,Bulletin of the Atomic
Scientists, Vol. 62, No. 4, 2006, p. 68.
Atwan, Abdel Bari, e Secret History of al Qaeda, Berkeley, Calif.: University of
California Press, 2006.
Berrebi, Claude, and Darius Lakdawalla, “How Does Terrorism Risk Vary Across
Space and Time? An Analysis Based on the Israeli Experience,Defence and Peace
Economics, Vol. 18, No. 2, 2007, pp. 113–131.
Bloom, Mia, Dying to Kill: the Allure of Suicide Terror, New York: Columbia
University Press, 2007.
Bueno de Mesquita, Ethan, and Eric S. Dickson, “e Propaganda of the Deed:
Terrorism, Counterterrorism, and Mobilization,American Journal of Political
Science, Vol. 51, No. 2, 2007, pp. 364–381.
Calle, Luis de la, and Ignacio Sánchez-Cuenca, “e Production of Terrorist
Violence: Analyzing Target Selection in the IRA and ETA,” unpublished, 2007.
Carley, Kathleen M., “Organizational Learning and Personnel Turnover,
Organization Science, Vol. 3, No. 1, 1992, pp. 2046.
Carley, Kathleen M., and David M. Svoboda, “Modeling Organizational
Adaptation as a Simulated Annealing Process,Sociological Methods & Research,
Vol. 25, No. 1, 1996, pp. 138–168.
242 Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces Together
Carter, Josh, “Transcending the Nuclear Framework: Deterrence and Compellence
as Counter-Terrorism Strategies,Low Intensity Conflict and Law Enforcement, Vol.
10, No. 2, Summer 2001, pp. 84–102.
Casebeer, William, and Troy omas, “Deterring Violent Non-State Actors in
the New Millennium,Strategic Insights, Vol. 1, No. 10, December 2002. As of
January 5, 2009:
http://www.ccc.nps.navy.mil/si/dec02/terrorism2.asp
Chai, Sun-Ki, “An Organizational Economics eory of Antigovernment
Violence,Comparative Politics, Vol. 26, No. 1, 1993, pp. 99–110.
Combating Terrorism Center, Harmony and Disharmony: Exploiting al-Qaida’s
Organizational Vulnerabilities, West Point, N.Y.: U.S. Military Academy, 2006.
Connor, Robert, Defeating the Modern Asymmetric reat, Monterey, Calif.: Naval
Postgraduate School, 2002.
Cragin, Kim, and Sara A. Daly, e Dynamic Terrorist reat: An Assessment of
Group Motivations and Capabilities in a Changing World, Santa Monica, Calif.:
RAND Corporation, 2004. As of January 5, 2009:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/monograph_reports/MR1782/
Crelinsten, Ronald D., “e Internal Dynamics of the FLQ During the October
Crisis of 1970,” in David C. Rapoport, ed., Inside Terrorist Organizations, New
York: Columbia University Press, 1988, pp. 5989.
Crenshaw [Hutchinson], Martha, “e Concept of Revolutionary Terrorism,e
Journal of Conflict Resolution, Vol. 16, No. 3, September 1972, pp. 383–396.
Crenshaw, Martha “e Causes of Terrorism,Comparative Politics, Vol. 13, No.
4, July 1981, pp. 379–399.
———,An Organizational Approach to the Analysis of Political Terrorism,
Orbis, Vol. 29, No. 3, Fall 1985, pp. 465489.
———, “e Psychology of Political Terrorism,” in Margaret G. Hermann,
ed., Political Psychology, San Francisco, Calif.: Jossey-Bass Publishers, 1986, pp.
379413.
———,e Logic of Terrorism: Terrorist Behavior as a Product of Strategic
Choice,” in Walter Reich, ed., Origins of Terrorism: Psychologies, Ideologies,
eologies, States of Mind, Washington, D.C.: Woodrow Wilson International
Center for Scholars, 1990, pp. 7–24.
———, ed., Terrorism in Context, University Park, Pa.: Pennsylvania State
University Press, 1995.
———,e Psychology of Terrorism: An Agenda for the 21st Century,Political
Psychology, Vol. 21, No. 2, 2000, pp. 405420.
Organizational Decisionmaking by Terrorist Groups 243
———,eories of Terrorism: Instrumental and Organizational Approaches,
in David C. Rapoport, ed., Inside Terrorist Organizations, London: Frank Cass,
2001, pp. 13–31.
Davis, Paul K., and John Arquilla, inking About Opponent Behavior in Crisis and
Conflict: A Generic Model for Analysis and Group Discussion, Santa Monica, Calif.:
RAND Corporation, 1991. As of July 14, 2008:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/notes/N3322/
Davis, Paul K., and Brian Michael Jenkins, Deterrence & Influence in
Counterterrorism: A Component in the War on Al Qaeda, Santa Monica, Calif.:
RAND Corporation, 2002. As of July 14, 2008:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/monograph_reports/MR1619/
Decker, Warren, and Daniel Rainey, “Terrorism as Communication,” paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Speech Communication Association, New
York, November 1316, 1980. As of February 25, 2009:
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICDocs/data/ericdocs2sql/content_storage_
01/0000019b/80/35/f1/0b.pdf
Della Porta, Donatella, “Left-Wing Terrorism in Italy,” in Martha Crenshaw, ed.,
Terrorism in Context, University Park, Pa.: Pennsylvania State University Press,
1995, pp. 160–210.
Dingley, James, “e Bombing of Omagh, 15 August 1998: e Bombers,
eir Tactics, Strategy, and Purpose Behind the Incident,Studies in Conflict &
Terrorism, Vol. 24, 2001, pp. 451–465.
Dolnik, Adam, and Anjali Bhattacharjee, “Hamas: Suicide Bombings, Rockets,
or WMD?” Terrorism and Political Violence, Vol. 14, No. 3, Autumn 2002, pp.
109–128.
Don, Bruce W., David R. Frelinger, Scott Gerwehr, Eric Landree, and Brian
A. Jackson, Network Technologies for Networked Terrorists: Assessing the Value of
Information and Communications Technologies to Modern Terrorist Organizations,
Santa Monica, Calif.: RAND Corporation, 2007. As of July 14, 2008:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/technical_reports/TR454/
Drake, C. J. M., “e Role of Ideology in Terrorists’ Target Selection,Terrorism
and Political Violence, Vol. 5, No. 4, 1993, pp. 253–265.
———, Terrorists’ Target Selection, New York: St. Martin’s Press, 1998.
Drakos, Konstantinos, “e Size of Under-Reporting Bias in Recorded
Transnational Terrorist Activity,J.R. Statist. Soc A, Vol. 170, No. 4, October 1,
2007, pp. 909–921.
Drakos, Konstantinos, and Andreas Gofas, “e Devil You Know But Are Afraid
to Face: Underreporting Bias and Its Distorting Effects on the Study of Terrorism,
Journal of Conflict Resolution, Vol. 50, No. 5, October 1, 2006, pp. 714–735.
244 Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces Together
Dugan, Laura, Gary Lafree, and Alex R. Piquero, “Testing a Rational Choice
Model of Airline Hijackings,Criminology, Vol. 43, No. 4, 2005, pp. 1031–1065.
Fleming, Marie, “Propaganda by the Deed: Terrorism and Anarchist eory in
Late Nineteenth-Century Europe,Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, Vol. 4, No.
1–4, 1980, pp. 1–23.
Gerges, Fawaz A., e Far Enemy: Why Jihad Went Global, New York: Cambridge
University Press, 2005.
Gurr, Ted Robert, “Terrorism in Democracies: Its Social and Political Bases,” in
Walter Reich, ed., Origins of Terrorism: Psychologies, Ideologies, eologies, States
of Mind, Washington, D.C.: Woodrow Wilson International Center for Scholars,
1990, pp. 86–102.
Gvineria, Gaga, “How Does Terrorism End?” in Paul K. Davis and Kim Cragin,
eds., Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces Together, Santa Monica,
Calif.: RAND Corporation, 2009. As of January 17, 2009:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/monographs/MG849/
Hayden, Nancy Kay, Terrifying Landscapes: A Study of Scientific Research into
Understanding Motivations of Non-State Actors to Acquire and/or Use Weapons of
Mass Destruction, Fort Belvoir, Va.: Defense reat Reduction Agency, 2007.
Helmus, Todd C., “Why and How Some People Become Terrorists,” in Paul K.
Davis and Kim Cragin, eds., Social Science for Counterterrorism: Putting the Pieces
Together, Santa Monica, Calif.: RAND Corporation, 2009. As of January 17,
2009:
http://www.rand.org/pubs/monographs/MG849/
Hoffman, Aaron, “Taking Credit for eir Work: When Groups Announce
Responsibility for Acts of Terror, 19681977,” paper presented at the annual
meeting of the International Studies Association, Montreal, Quebec, March 17,
2004.
Hoffman, Bruce, “e Modern Terrorist Mindset: Tactics, Targets and
Technologies,” Center for the Study of Terrorism and Political Violence, St.
Andrews. University, Scotland, October 1997. As of February 25, 2009:
http://www.ciaonet.org/wps/hob03/
———,e Myth of Grass-Roots Terrorism: Why Osama bin Laden Still
Matters,Foreign Affairs, May/June, 2008.
Hoffman, Bruce, and Gordon McCormick, “Terrorism, Signaling, and Suicide
Attack,Studies in Conflict & Terrorism, Vol. 27, No. 4, 2004, pp. 243–281.
Homeland Security Institute, Underlying Reasons for Success and Failure of Terrorist
Attacks: Selected Case Studies, Washington, D.C., 2007.
Horgan, John and Max Taylor, “e Provisional Irish Republican Army: Command
and Functional Structure,Terrorism and Political Violence, Vol. 9, No. 3, 1997.