1.2 Insect Outbreak Model: Spruce Budworm 11
budworm dispersal must be taken into effect; we shall discuss this aspect in detail in
Chapter 2, Volume II.
It is appropriate here to mention briefly the timescale with which this model is con-
cerned. An outbreak of budworm during which balsam fir trees are denuded of foliage
is about four years. The trees then die and birch trees take over. Eventually, in the com-
petition for nutrient, the fir trees will drive out the birch trees again. The timescale for
fir reforestation is of the order of 50 to 100 years. A full model would incorporate the
tree dynamics as well; see Ludwig et al. (1978). So, the model we have analysed here
is only for the short timescale, namely, that related to a budworm outbreak.
Hassell et al. (1999) have considered the original highly complex (more than 80
variables and parameters) multi-species interaction model involving large and small
larvae, fecundity, foliage mortality with and without the budworm, and other players
in the interaction. They show, using gross, but reasonably justifiable assumptions, that
the model can be simplified without losing the basic budworm–forest interaction. They
obtain several simplifications using asymptotic limits, eventually reducing the system
to three difference (as opposed to differential) equations for larvae, foliage and area
fraction of old trees. Analysis showed that the mechanism for oscillations could be
captured with only two equations. A more in depth analysis than we have done here, but
very much less than that by Hassell et al. (1999), is given in the very practical book on
modelling by Fowler (1997).
Catastrophes in Perception
Although the following does not strictly belong in a chapter on population dynamics, it
seemed appropriate to include it in the section where we discuss hysteresis and catas-
trophic change.
There is a series of now classic figures developed by Fisher (1967) which demon-
strate sudden changes in visual perception. Here we describe one of these picture series
and show that it is another example of a cusp catastrophe; it also exhibits an initial per-
ceptual hysteresis. We also present the results of an experiment carried out by the author
which confirms the hypothesis. The specific example we describe has been studied in
more depth by Zeeman (1982) and more generally by Stewart and Peregoy (1983).
The mind can be triggered, or moved in a major new thought or behavioural direc-
tion, by a vast variety of cues in ways we cannot yet hope to describe in any biologi-
cal detail. A step in this direction, however, is to be able to describe the phenomenon
and demonstrate its existence via example. Such sudden changes in perception and be-
haviour are quite common in psychology and therapy.
4
The series of pictures, numbered 1 to 8 are shown in Figure 1.8.
4
An interesting example of a major change in a patient undergoing psychoanalysis treated by the French
psychoanalyst Marie Claire Boons is described by Zeeman (1982). The case involved a frigid woman whom
she had been treating for two years without much success. ‘One day the patient reported dreaming of a frozen
rabbit in her arms, which woke and said hello. The patient’s words were “un lapin congel
´
e,” meaning a frozen
rabbit, to which the psychoanalyst slowly replied “la pin con gel
´
e?” This is a somewhat elaborate pun. The
word “pin” is the French slang for penis, the female “la” makes it into the clitoris, “con” is the French slang
for the female genitals, and “gel
´
e” means both frozen and rigid. The surprising result was that the patient did
not respond for 20 minutes and the next day came back cured. Apparently that evening she had experienced
her first orgasm ever with her husband.’