to speak, sandwiched between the Atlantic Ocean and the Congo Basin in the
south and the Sahara and Sahel in the north, and spans the continent from the
Atlantic Ocean in the west to the escarpment of the Ethiopian Plateau in the east.
To a considerable extent, there are also linguistic correlates of the above
external boundaries. That is, the area excludes regions which are more
homogeneous in linguistic-genealogical terms, namely the Saharan spread
zone in the north covered today by Berber, Saharan, and Arabic; the spread
zone in the south colonized by Narrow Bantu; and finally the Ethiopian Plateau
in the east dominated by Cushitic and Ethio-Semitic (see chapter 7; see Nichols
1992 for the general concept of a ‘‘spread zone’’).
Regarding the internal profile of the area, o ne needs to distinguish between
different types of languages and language groups constituting it. That is, not
all linguistic lineages concerned are involved to the same degree in certain
distribution patterns of linguistic features.
The core of the area is formed by the following language families: Mande,
Kru, Gur, Kwa, Benue-Congo (excluding Narrow Bantu), Adamawa-Ubangi,
Bongo-Bagirmi,
1
and Moru-Ma ngbetu. The two easternmost families of
Niger-Congo, Benue-Congo and Adamawa-Ubangi, as well as the two Central
Sudanic families, Bongo-Bagirmi and Moru-Ma ngbetu, will be shown to hold
a particularly prominent position in the core group and form again a compact
geographical block.
Some lineages, which in geographical terms are all peripheral but still
adjacent to the core, display an ambiguous behavior regarding linguistic
commonalities with this area. These lineages are Atlantic, Dogon, Songhay,
Chadic, Ijoid, Narrow Bantu, and Nilotic.
The above remarks suggest that genealogical language groups to be
considered in this chapter are usually low-level units, called here ‘‘families,’’
ignoring the four super-groups proposed by Greenberg (1963). Reasons for
taking such smaller genealogical units as the reference of continental sampling
will be postponed until section 5.4.2. Suffice it to say here that my approach has
the advantage that a greater variety of languages will have to be included and
no relevant genealogical group for which data are available is unduly omitted.
Clearly, if this breakdown were to be transferred into a genealogical clas-
sification this would be a far more splitting one. The present schema, which
does not refer to groups like Khoisan, Nilo-Saharan, and Niger-Kordofanian,
2
should, however, not be viewed as an alternative classification proposal; to
develop such a classification woul d be an endeavor in its own right. Low-level
sampling is warranted by the particular topic of thi s chapter, which must
consider the possibility that certain types of linguistic commonalities, when
involving genealogical entities that are not yet based on solid evidence, may
well have an explanation other than common inheritance, inter alia, one in
terms of areal contact.
Tom Gu
¨
ldemann152