References 239
305. D. C. Geary, C. C. Bow-Thomas, L. Fan, and R. S. Siegler, Even before
formal instruction, Chinese children outperform American children
in mental additi on. Cognitive Development 8 (1993), 517–529.
306. D. C. Geary and S. C. Brown, Cognitive addition: Strategy choice and
speed-of-processing differences in gifted, normal and mathematically
disabled children. Developmental Psychology 27 (1991), 39 8–406.
307. R. Gelman and C. R. Gallistel, The Child’s Understanding of
Number. Harvard University Press, 1978.
308. R. Gelman an d C. R. Gallistel, Lan guage and the origin of numerical
concepts. Science 306 (2004), 441–443.
309. S. P. Gennari, S. A. Sloman, B. C. Malt, and W. T. Fitch, Motion events
in language and cognition. Cognition 83, no. 1 (2002), 49–79.
310. D. Gentner, M. Imai, an d L. Boroditsky. As time goes by: Evidence
for two systems i n processing space!time metaphors. Language and
Cognitive Processes 17 no. 5 (2002), 537–56 5. DOI: 10.1080/01690960
14300031 7.
311. R. W. Gi bbs, Why many concepts are metaphorical. Cognition 61
(1996), 309–319.
312. R. Goebel, D. Lin d en, H. L anfermann, F. Zanella, and W. Singer,
Functional imaging of mirror and inverse reading reveals s eparate
coactivated networks for occulomotion and spatial transformations.
Neuroreport 9 (1998), 71 3–719.
313. S. Goldin-Meadow, Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us
Think. Harvard University Press, C ambridge, MA, 2003.
314. S. Goldin-Meadow, M. W. Alibali, and R. B. Church, Transitions in
concept acquisi tion: Using the hand to read the mi nd. Psychological
Review 100 (1993), 279–297.
315. S. Goldin-Meadow, S. Kim, and M. Sin ger, What the teacher’s hands
tell the student’s mind about math. J. Ed. Psychology, 91 (1999), 720–
730.
316. S. Goldin-Meadow, H. N usbaum, S. D. Kelly, and S. Wagner, Ex-
plaining math: Gesturing lightens the load . Psychological Science 12
(2001), 516–522.
317. S. Goldin-Meadow and M. A. Singer, From children’s hands to adul ts’
ears: Gesture’s role in teaching and learning. Developmental Psychol-
ogy 39 (200 3), 509–520.
318. A. Gopnik, The Philosophical Baby: What Children’s Minds Tell
Us About Truth, Love, and the Meaning of Life. Farrar, Straus
and Giroux, 2009, 28 8 pp.
319. J. A. Gottfried, F. Sancar, and A. Chatterjee, Acquired mirror writ-
ing and readi ng: evidence for reflected graphemic representations.
Neuropsychologia 41 (2003), 96–107. DOI:10.1016/S0028-3 932(02)
00130-6.
320. E. L. Grigorenko, Developmental dyslexia: An update on genes,
brains, a nd environments, J. Chi ld Psych. and Psychiatry 42, no. 1
(2001), 91–125. DOI: 10.1017/S0 02196 3001006564.
321. C. G. Gross and M. H. Bornstein, Left and right in science and Art,
Leonardo 11 (19 78), 29–38.
322. M. A. K. Halliday, Aspects of sociolinguistic research, UN-
ESCO, Paris, 1974. A survey paper prepare d for Sympo sium
on the Interactions Between Linguistics and Mathematical
SHADOWS OF THE TRUTH VER. 0.813 23-DEC-2010/7:19
c
ALEXANDRE V. BOROVIK