areal feature: Appleyard (1989) discusses relative verbs in focus constructions;
Tosco (1994b) deals with case marking; and Tosco (1996) with extended verb
paradigms in the Gurage-Sidamo subarea, one of the subareas of the ELA
proposed by Zaborski ( 1991).
Tosco (2000b) denies the existence of the ELA because of the genetic
relatedness of Ethio-Semitic and Cushitic languages, the unilateral diffusion
from Cushitic to Ethio-Semitic and the occurrence of features in related lan-
guages, which do not belong to the ELA. Four recent papers, namely Bender
(2003), Crass (2002), Crass and Bisang (2004), and Zaborski (2003) favor the
existence of a linguistic area. Bender (2003) argues against Tosco (2000b)and
tries to extend the ELA by testing a number of Nilo-Saharan languages using a
selection of Ferguson’s features. Crass (2002) discusses two phonological fea-
tures in detail; in Crass and Bisang (2004) the discussion is extended to features
such as word order, converbs, and ideophones verbalized by the verb ‘to say.’
Zaborski (2003) presents the most extended list, including twenty-eight features
which he considers to be valid for a macro-area including Ethiopia, Eritrea,
Djibouti, Somalia, and parts of Sudan, Kenya, and even Tanzania and Uganda.
Finally, Hayward (1991) deals with patterns of lexicalization shared by the three
Ethiopian languages Amharic (Semitic), Oromo (Cushitic), and Gamo (Omotic).
According to Hayward (1991: 140), these lexicalizations reinforce ‘‘the very real
cultural unity of Ethiopia’’ (see also Hayward 2000b).
The ELA is consider ed to be composed of several subareas. Leslau (1952,
1959) describes change in Ethio-Semitic languages induced by contact with
neighboring Highland East Cushitic languages. Sasse ( 1986) deals with the
Sagan area in the southwest of Ethiopia, and Zaborski (1991: 125ff.) gives a list
of seven subareas being composed of ‘‘smaller contact and interference units’’
which he extends to nine by adding a Kenyan and a Ta nzanian subarea
(Zaborski 2003: 64).
7.2.1 Phonological features
Ferguson’s phonological features are listed in table 7.1 .
Ferguson’s li st has been criticized in most of the later publications. Zaborski
(1991: 124, footnote 3) considers only P3 and ‘‘with reservations’’ P2 to be
‘‘really areal.’’ Zaborski (2003: 62) lists four phonological features. Besides P3
and P6, Zaborski argues that ‘‘labialized conson ants are frequent [and that]
some palatalized consonants are innovations.’’ Tosco (2000b: 341ff.) is of the
opinion that P1, P2, P3, and P5 are genetically inherited within Afroasiatic, that
P4, P7, and P8 are restricted to one or two language families, and that P6 is
widespread in both Afroasiatic and Nilo-Saharan. According to Bender (n.d.),
P2 and P6 are typological features, P5 is too limited and P8 ‘‘is vacuous
because consonant clusters are rare.’’ P1, P3, P4, and P7, however, are ‘‘fairly
Joachim Crass and Ronny Meyer230