31
13 IT’S A MYTH THAT CAPITAL LETTERS ARE INHERENTLY HARD TO READ
Figure13.2 shows an example of the saccade and fixation pattern. The black dots
are the fixations and the curved lines are the saccade movements.
Fortunately these saccades are really fast so you are not blind for long.
They are so fast that you don’t even realize they are happening.
FIGURE13.2 An example of a saccade and fixation pattern
We use peripheral vision when we read
A saccade spans about seven to nine letters, but our perceptual span is actually double
that. In 1996, Kenneth Goodman found that we use peripheral vision to see what comes
next when we read. We read ahead about 15 letters at a time, viewing the characters to
the right (assuming we’re reading left to right), although now and then a saccade jumps
us backward and we reread a group of letters. Although we read ahead about 15 letters
at a time, we only get the meaning for part of that span. We pick up the semantic cues of
letters 1 through 7, but merely recognize letters 8 through 15.
Reading music is similar to reading text
People who read music fluently use the same saccades, fixations, and reading ahead of
15 “letters” that they do when reading text.
SO, IS ALL CAPITALS HARDER TO READ THEN?
We do actually read uppercase letters more slowly, but only because we don’t see them
as often. Most of what we read is in mixed case, so we’re used to it. If you practice read-
ing text in all capital letters, you’ll eventually read that text as fast as you read mixed
case. This doesn’t mean you should start using capital letters for all your text. Since
peole are unused to reading that way, it will slow them down. And these days, text in all
caps is perceived as “shouting” (Figure13.3).
*DOUBLE CHECK THE LOCATION YOU ARE SIGNING UP FOR
*BE SURE TO ASSIGN AND UNDO CODE (Codes are case sensitive)
*CHECK YOUR EMAIL FREQUENTELY - CLASS INFO WILL BE COMMUNICATED VIA EMAIL
FIGURE13.3 We perceive uppercase letters as shouting, but they aren’t inherently harder to read
sacca
e spans a
out seven to nine
etters,
ut our perceptua
span is actua
y
ou
e
that. In 1
6, Kenneth
oodman found that we use peripheral vision to see what comes
next when we read. We read ahead about 15 letters at a time, viewin
the characters to
the right (assuming we’re reading left to right), although now and then a saccade jumps
us backward and we reread a group o
letters. Although we read ahead about 15 letters
t a time, we onl
et the meanin
or part o
that span. We pick up the semantic cues o
letters 1 throu
h 7, but merel
reco
nize letters 8 throu
h 15
People who read music fluentl
use the same saccades, fixations, and readin
ahead of
5 “letters” that they do when reading text.
1