1 2 1 -
Winter used a 'multiple strategy' approach (Winter, 1990) to collect evidence about 11- and 12-
year-olds' perceptions of science and their developing process skills. She observed the children at work in
her science classes and wrote accounts of her lessons in a research diary. She carried out semi-structured,
tape-recorded interviews with children, and kept samples of their work on scientific topics. In addition,
she asked some children to keep diaries in which they made notes of their comments and impressions
about their science lessons. Winter's methods were participant observation, interviewing, and analysis of
children's work. Her data were her written diary record of her observations; transcripts of the interviews;
samples of children's work; and the diaries they kept. This was the evidence she used to support her
arguments and recommendations. You will find all of these methods of gathering evidence discussed in
subsequent sections of this Handbook.
Winter found that children's practical work benefited most when they were allowed to work in
friendship groups, and when they were encouraged to take responsibility for managing their own group
work. On the basis of her evidence Winter demonstrated that, for her children, the original assumption
that practical experiences enhance and consolidate children's learning needed to be modified. She found
that children make more progress when they are encouraged to take control of their own learning, and
when they are allowed to work in friendship groups. Contrary to her original assumption, simply giving
them practical experience was not enough. Very often in educational research new theories and
hypotheses arise in the course of collecting information to answer a particular research question. Where
this happens we talk about theories being 'grounded in the data', to describe the way in which some
theories can arise directly from practical experiences and observations. You will find more on 'grounded
theory' in sub-section 5.3.
THE QUALITATIVE/QUAN TITATIVE DEST INCT ION
So far we have only talked about 'theories', 'evidence', and 'data' in very general terms. It is
customary, however, to make a distinction between methods of collecting evidence that give rise to
qualitative data and those which give rise to quantitative data.
Hugh Coolican explains the difference between qualitative and quantitative approaches as follows:
'Quantification' means to measure on some numerical basis … Whenever we
count or categorise we quantify. … A qualitative approach, by contrast,
emphasises meanings, experiences … descriptions and so on. Raw data will be
exactly what people have said (in interview or recorded conversation) or a
description of what has been observed.
(Coolican, 1990, pp. 36-7)
Traditional experimental approaches to educational research use quantification and measurement
to examine the contributions of different factors to the behaviours studied. So suppose you were
interested in comparing the effects on children's reading performance of two different reading schemes,
one based on a phonic approach, and one based on a visual word-recognition approach. In an
experimental study, it would be essential to choose a reliable and unbiased measure of reading
performance, such as scores from a standardized reading test, which could be used with both schemes.
The measure should either provide a numerical score for each child, or tell you how many children fall
within each of a set of defined categories. Data of this sort can be interpreted with the aid of statistics to
allow you to make a quantitative comparison between the two schemes.
The use of quantitative techniques is not restricted to experimental studies. Data from
observations, interviews and questionnaires can also be quantified provided that it is structured
appropriately. Examples of this type of study are discussed in Section 5.
We do not deal with the statistical analysis of quantitative data in this Part. However, the further
reading lists at the end of this section and at the end of Section 5 contain a selection of books about
quantitative experimental techniques and statistical analysis.
Quantitative techniques are extremely useful if you want to compare things like people's test
scores or measures of performance under different conditions, or how people behave under different
conditions. Quantitative experimental techniques have the disadvantage that, to make the resulting data