
Interactivity
87
2002: 93; original emphasis).
That
people
tend
to
gravitate towards media
that
confirm
or
reinforce
their
own
attitudes counteracts
the
hypothetic
breadth
of
opinion
that
the
internet
can
be
understood
to
channel
to
people.
When
these considerations are translated
into
the
frame of
online
interactive
debate we
can
see
that
there
is
a
strong
tendency
for
homophonous
opinion
to
emerge.
In
his study
of
political discussion boards Wilhelm found
that
70
per
cent
of
the
messages posted could be considered
homophonous
in
so far
as
they
expressed strong
or
moderate
support
for
the
dominant
position
of
the
board as a whole (2000:
86-105).
This finding will surprise
neither
sociologists
nor
political theorists
as
it
is
a variation
upon
well-documented
tendencies towards social centripetalism. The
tendency
for conflicting views
to
be marginalized
and
ultimately
'outsourced'
to
other
forums
is
in
line
with
Noelle-Neumann's description of
the
'spiral of silence'. Briefly, Noelle-
Neumann
found
that
the
further away from
what
they
perceived as
the
common
opinion
people were,
the
less likely
they
were
to
express
their
views,
whereas
the
closer
they
perceived themselves
to
be
the
more
likely
they
were
to
express
them
and
be vocal
in
their
support
(Noelle-Neumann 1984, cited
in
Sunstein 2001: 68). Thus,
although
'anyone
can
say
anything',
in
reality it
appears
that
they
mostly say similar things.
However,
homophily
is
not
the
same
thing
as reciprocity,
which
brings us
to
the
second problem
with
online
debates,
namely
the
nature
of
dialogue
online. This issue
is
covered
in
more
detail
in
Part Three,
but
here
it
is
important
to
bring
out
the
qualities of
interaction
which
affect
the
circulation
or
quality of information.
In
the
first instance
there
is
the
question
of
'interactional
noise'
. Even
the
most
committed
cyberspace advocates
acknowledge
that
there
are significant interactional problems
in
online
discussions, problems
which
tend
to
hinge
on
the
lack
of
visual
and
aural cues
which
serve
to
narrow
the
indexical range
of
utterances
and
reduce
the
probability of misunderstandings. This paucity
of
cues,
when
taken against
the
background
of
overload, represents a difficulty for
internet
debaters.
In
some
ways
it
is
the
offline
equivalent
of
carrying
on
a
potentially
inflammatory
discussion
in
a
loud
room. The possibilities for misunderstand-
ings where responses are rapidly composed
and
delivered
at
the
click of a
mouse,
and
the
likelihood
that
many
responses will simply be ignored,
both
serve
to
increase
the
confrontational
possibilities of
online
debate. However,
'(w)hen
technology
provides both speed and
anonymity,
it produces a
concoction
that
can
spark hostility
and
attack' (Tannen 1999: 252, emphasis
added)
and
the
semi-anonymous
nature
of
online
interaction
may
be
at
the
root
of
noted
hostility.
As
Tannen
argues, disagreements escalate
into
confrontations
where mechanisms
of
resolution are absent
or
weak.
In
contemporary
society
the
chief
means
by
which
reconciliations are achieved
is
the
recovery of
the
personal: we seek
out
face-to-face interaction
in
order
to
overcome disagreements; getting
to
know
someone
is
a
time-honoured
method
of diffusing tension. Online, where, if
postmodern
psychologists
and
post-structuralist theorists (e.g. Poster, Turkle) are
to
believed (see
Chapter
9),
anonymity
facilitates
identity
play
and
self-reconstruction,
there
is
no
'other'
co-present
to
enable this negotiation.
As
Tannen
points
out
, for some of
the
internet
users she studied .email
is
like writing
in
a journal; you're alone
with