timate the mental qualities of the apes—and by extension, of the early
hominids. Apes show remarkable, if limited, powers of intuitive rea-
soning, as well as a striking ability to communicate their emotional
states and to understand the motivations of other individuals. They even
develop local ‘‘cultural’’ traditions involving the transmission from one
generation to the next of learned behaviors such as cracking nuts on
stone anvils and ‘‘fishing’’ with sticks in termite mounds. Indeed, many
primatologists think that the capacity for culture in this restricted sense
is a basic great-ape trait, and if so, we have even greater reason to believe
that the apes can give us a general picture of the apparently quite im-
pressive intellectual starting point of our own lineage.
But whether or not this turns out to be the case, it is still important
not to view early hominids simply as junior-league versions of ourselves:
implicitly, creatures striving to become us. Equally clearly, these ancient
relatives did business in their own unique ways and weren’t apes, either.
But one of the ways in which A. afarensis and species like it seem to have
been significantly closer to apes than to us was the speed with which
they developed from infancy to maturity. Young apes grow up much
more quickly than young humans do; a male chimpanzee is reproduc-
tively mature at about six to seven years of age, for example, whereas a
male human takes twice as long, or longer. This prolonged maturation
process—which, it is important to note, extends the period of social
learning—expresses itself among other things in the rate at which the
permanent teeth erupt. It has been shown that the earliest hominids
matured quite rapidly, at rates probably comparable with those of apes.
A relatively rapid developmental process may, indeed, have character-
ized hominids until quite a late stage in their evolution.
Australopithecus afarensis, though a good example of its group, is
only the best known of several species that were traditionally classified
in the subfamily Australopithecinae of the family Hominidae. This sub-
family is nowadays implicitly taken to include all of the extinct homi-
nids, with the exception of those allocated to the genus Homo—which
raises problems of definition that have yet to be adequately addressed.
There is also, inevitably, some argument as to whether this group de-
serves the status of subfamily; there is, after all, debate even over the
level at which Hominidae itself should be recognized. Most scientists
thus currently prefer to use the more informal term ‘‘australopiths’’ for
this group, and we’ll do so here.
The australopiths have been known since 1924, when the first such
specimen, described under the name of Australopithecus africanus, was
found in a lime quarry in South Africa. This specimen consisted of the
On Their Own Two Feet
51