could only take five cores per day. Therefore the most upstream part of the
estuary was sampled in May, the middle in June and the most downstream
sites in August. The sampling showed a positive correlation between the
sodium content of newly laid down sediment and increasing distance
downstream. Unfortunately, however, the change in sodium content was
actually caused by seasonal variation in salinity that was low throughout the
entire estuary in spring due to groundwater run-off and increased as the
year progressed (and the scientist did not know this). Subsequent work
where the entire estuary was sampled on the same day found no difference
in the sodium content of the sediments, so the correlation between distance
downstream and sodium content was an artifact of the sampling of different
places being confounded in time. This is an example of a common problem,
and you are likely to find similar cases in many published scientific papers
and reports.
4.2.3 The need for independent samples in mensurative
experiments
Frequently researchers have to accurately describe relatively large areas or
objects as part of a mensurative experiment. For example, annual sedimen-
tation rates in glacier-fed lakes are used to reconstruct up-valley glacier
activity and thereby model environmental change. Sediment traps are
deployed at various locations in these lakes, and the mean particle size,
quantity and sediment flux are calculated. There is an obvious need to
replicate the sampling – that is, to independently measure sedimentation
rate in more than one place.
If you only sampled sediment in one trap at one place (Figure 4.3(a)) the
results would not be a good indication of the sediment accreting across the
whole lake. The sampling needs to be replicated, but there is little value in
repeatedly sampling one small area (e.g. by taking several samples under
“*****” in Figure 4.3(b)) because this still will not give an accurate indication
of variation in sediment rate and composition across the whole lake
(although it may give a very accurate indication of conditions in that
particular part of the lake sampled). This sort of sampling is one aspect of
what Hurlbert (1984) called pseudoreplication, which is still a very com-
mon flaw in a lot of scientific research. The replicates are “pseudo”–sham
or unreal – because they are unlikely to truly describe what is occurring
32 Introductory concepts of experimental design